I Have Something in Common with Marilyn Monroe—and You Might, Too

Image may contain Finger Human Person and Furniture
Marilyn Monroe had synesthesia, a sensory condition that has led to a broad reconsideration of perception in general.Photograph by Elliot Erwitt / Magnum

Marilyn Monroe had a condition called synesthesia, a kind of sensory or cognitive fusion in which things seen, heard, smelled, felt, or tasted stimulate a totally unrelated sense—so that music can be heard or food tasted in colors, for instance. Monroe’s first husband, Jim Dougherty, told Norman Mailer about “evenings when all Norma Jean served were peas and carrots. She liked the colors. She has that displacement of the senses which others take drugs to find. So she is like a lover of rock who sees vibrations when he hears sounds,” Mailer recounted, in his 1973 biography of Monroe.

I have synesthesia, too. The condition, which has been described in literature for centuries but only recently studied scientifically, takes many forms. The word comes from the Greek “syn,” or union, and “aesthesis” or sensation, literally meaning the joining of the senses—a kind of neurological crosstalk. Experts have so far chronicled more than a hundred variations. About four per cent of people are believed to have at least one variation; some have many. We’re called synesthetes.

I see numbers in colors, which is one of the more common forms of synesthesia. For me, three is a sunny yellow, four is bright red, five is a brilliant green, six is pale blue, seven is royal blue, eight is muddy brown, and so on. I do Sudoku puzzles by colors, not by the shapes of numbers. I remember phone numbers by colors, too. If the colors go together, I’ll never forget the number. If they clash, it’s almost impossible to recall. When I went to my home town, Ann Arbor, for my thirtieth high-school reunion, I picked up the phone and called a friend I hadn’t seen in decades—on a number I remembered (and still do). I hate the number nineteen: one is white, nine is black. It’s like good and evil in one number. It makes me shudder.

“Synesthesia is a genuine phenomenon, and people who have it are actually experiencing the world differently,” Dr. David Eagleman, a neuroscientist at Stanford, told me. “It alters perceptions.” Synesthetes tend to have better memories, for one thing. “They have an added way of remembering something,” he said. I actually have a rotten memory—except that I’m a whiz at numbers.

Research into synesthesia has led to a broad reconsideration of perception in general, according to Richard Cytowic, a professor of neurology at George Washington University. “From the scientific point of view, it’s caused a paradigm shift. Perception may be due for a redefinition,” he told me. “Our eyes see, yes, but vision can apparently also hear. Tactile receptors can also taste. If synesthesia research continues this way, we may find that we all have a little bit of it in us.”

Eagleman and Cytowic chronicle varieties of the condition in their book “Wednesday Is Indigo Blue: Discovering the Brain of Synesthesia.” “A person with synesthesia might feel the flavor of food on her fingertips, sense the letter ‘J’ as shimmering magenta, or the number ‘5’ as emerald green, hear and taste her husband's voice as buttery golden brown,” they write. The authors selected the title because some synesthetes see time in colors. Others see months or even years spatially, in three dimensions, as if laid out in the air around them in different directions. One of the more unusual forms involves taste, like the synesthete who said that chicken breasts taste blue.

The actor Geoffrey Rush has spatial synesthesia. He told the synesthesia expert Maureen Seaberg that, when he was a child, “the days of the week just instantly had strong color associations. Monday for me is kind of a pale blue, and I kind of imagine the day like that. Tuesday is acid green, Wednesday is a deep purple-y darkish color. Friday’s got maroon, and Saturday is white, and Sunday is sort of pale yellow.”

Artists, musicians, and writers are commonly synesthetes, Eagleman and Cytowic claim. Billy Joel and Pharrell Williams see music in color. Williams once described his blockbuster tune “Happy” as being yellow with accents of mustard and sherbet orange. Joel told Psychology Today that strong rhythms run, for him, in vivid reds and golds. Soft and slower melodies flow in blue and green tones. For Duke Ellington, the jazz maestro, a D on his colleague’s baritone saxophone might be a deep blue, the texture of burlap, while a G on another musician’s alto sax might be light blue with a satiny finish, according to the biography “Sweet Man: The Real Duke Ellington,” by Don George. In the eighteenth century, the Hungarian composer Franz Liszt was known to command his orchestras to play “a little bluer,” or to demand, “That is a deep violet, please, depend on it! Not so rose!”

Thanks to the advent of neuroimaging, scientists now believe that, physiologically, synesthesia is produced when regions of the brain communicate with one another. “Think of it like two countries with porous borders,” Eagleman said. “In most brains, they stay separate. But, in the synesthete’s brain, they communicate.”

Evidence indicates that synesthesia is genetic, Cytowic told me. Vladimir Nabokov, the author of “Lolita,” was a synesthete. So were his mother and son. “I have this rather freakish gift of seeing letters in color. It’s called color hearing,” he told the BBC, in 1962, before the serious study of synesthesia began, in the nineteen-seventies. Asked about the colors of his initials, he replied, “V is a kind of pale, transparent pink. I think it's called, technically, quartz pink: this is one of the closest colors that I can connect with the V. And the N, on the other hand, is a greyish-yellowish oatmeal color.” In his memoir, “Speak, Memory,” Nabokov describes the letters associated with shades of green. “T” is pistachio. “P” is the color of an unripe apple. “F” is the shade of an alder leaf. But Nabokov’s son, Dmitri, saw letters in completely different colors. That’s common, even between identical twins with synesthesia, Cytowic told me.

A couple of years ago, I discovered that one of my colleagues was a synesthete. Her numbers were totally different colors from mine. For her, three is red, four is lemon yellow, five is sky blue, six is light purple, seven is chocolate brown. (It’s common for synesthetes to have very specific shades associated with letters or numbers or food or days of the week.) She also sees the alphabet in colors, as do I, but hers are more vivid and varied. “A” is eggplant purple. “C” is light pink. “L” is clear and glassy, more like a texture than a color. And “S” is blue. She can remember names by colors but can get confused by spelling if a name has both a “P” and an “R.” Both, for her, are shades of green.

If you think you might be a synesthete, too, there’s a test that Eagleman and Cytowic devised, on the synesthete.org research Web site. It’s impossible to fake, Cytowic told me. “Synesthetes have a propensity to be unbelievably particular in their experience,” he said. The color wheel offers more than sixteen million shades. One test requires matching letters and colors—three times. For a synesthete, it’s easy to repeatedly identify the same color among millions of choices. Fakers can’t.